Menu
Psyche
DonateNewsletter
SIGN IN
Abstract image of blurred multi-coloured lights, including red, yellow, green and orange circles, on a dark background.

Photo by Mordolff/Getty

i

‘Moral molecules’ – a new theory of what goodness is made of

Photo by Mordolff/Getty

by Oliver Scott Curry, Mark Alfano, Mark Brandt & Christine Pelican + BIO

Save

Share

Post

Email

Seven basic moral elements could be the building blocks of morality, recombining as needed to form new moral molecules

How many moral values are there? What are they? What does it take to be a morally good person? Over the centuries, philosophers, theologians and others have offered no shortage of answers to these questions.

Plato argued that there were four virtues: temperance, courage, wisdom and justice. Aristotle said that there were 14 – including generosity, magnanimity, friendliness, honesty and modesty. The ancient Egyptians promised to refrain from the 42 sins of Maat. Judaism distilled 613 commandments into the famous 10, including prohibitions against murder, adultery, stealing and lying. Christianity added faith, hope and charity to Plato’s list to make seven heavenly virtues (along with seven corresponding deadly sins). Confucianism endorses virtues such as filial piety, trustworthiness and wisdom. The 20 laws of Aberewa, a spirit figure of the Akan of Ghana, encourage paying your debts and accepting requests for help, and discourage disobeying chiefs and ‘carrying firewood in bundles into town’. The philosopher W D Ross identified seven prima facie moral duties: fidelity, reparation, gratitude, non-maleficence, beneficence, self-improvement and justice. More recently, psychologists have argued that there are variously three (community, autonomy, divinity), or four (unity, respect, equality, proportionality) or five to six (care, fairness, loyalty, authority, purity and perhaps liberty) basic moral principles.

Which of these many philosophies and models offers the best guide to goodness? And can we do better? A typical problem with previous attempts to map the moral domain is that they are not based on an underlying theory of morality, and hence they are ad hoc and unsystematic. Recent developments in the scientific study of morality address this problem and offer a more rigorous and expansive approach.

Research is converging on the idea that morality is a collection of rules for promoting cooperation – rules that help us work together, get along, keep the peace and promote the common good. The basic idea is that humans are social animals who have lived together in groups for millions of years. During this time, we have been surrounded by opportunities for cooperation – for mutually beneficial social interaction – and we have evolved and invented a range of ways of unlocking these benefits. These cooperative strategies come in different shapes and sizes: instincts, intuitions, inventions, institutions. Together, they motivate our cooperative behaviour and provide the criteria by which we evaluate the behaviour of others. And it is these cooperative strategies that philosophers and others have called ‘morality’.

This theory of ‘morality as cooperation’ relies on the mathematical analysis of cooperation provided by game theory – the branch of maths that is used to describe situations in which the outcome of one’s decisions depends on the decisions made by others. Game theory distinguishes between competitive ‘zero-sum’ interactions or ‘games’, where one player’s gain is another’s loss, and cooperative ‘nonzero-sum’ games, win-win situations in which both players benefit. What’s more, game theory tells us that there is not just one type of nonzero-sum game; there are many, with many different cooperative strategies for playing them. At least seven different types of cooperation have been identified so far, and each one explains a different type of morality.

Morality is a system in which basic moral ‘elements’ combine to form complex moral ‘molecules’

The first, kin selection, is the process whereby natural selection favours genes that deliver benefits to genetic relatives. Kin selection can explain why we love and care for our families. The second, mutualism, refers to situations in which we do better working together than we do by trying to go it alone – there is strength and safety in numbers. Mutualism explains why we form clubs, cliques and coalitions, and why we value loyalty, unity and solidarity. The third, reciprocity, or conditional cooperation – helping those who help you, and harming those who harm you – is a viable strategy for cooperation when the return of benefits is uncertain (for example, when benefits are exchanged at different times). This strategy explains why we feel obliged to return favours and avenge injuries.

Even conflicts provide opportunities for cooperation, because competitors have a mutual interest in settling disputes without coming to blows. And they can do this in a number of ways: by signalling their fight-winning abilities through costly displays of power and prestige, including acts of heroism, bravery and generosity (which is the fourth type of cooperation); by humbly admitting defeat and deferring to powerful individuals (the fifth type); by dividing resources fairly rather than trying to monopolise them (sixth); and by recognising and respecting prior possession of resources – property – and refraining from theft (seventh).

Hence, seven types of cooperation explain seven types of morality: love, loyalty, reciprocity, heroism, deference, fairness and property rights. And so, according to this theory, it is morally good to: 1) love your family; 2) be loyal to your group; 3) return favours; 4) be heroic; 5) defer to superiors; 6) be fair; and 7) respect property. (And it is morally bad to: 1) neglect your family; 2) betray your group; 3) cheat; 4) be a coward; 5) disrespect authority; 6) be unfair; or 7) steal.) These morals are evolutionarily ancient, genetically distinct, psychologically discrete and cross-culturally universal.

The theory of ‘morality as cooperation’ explains, from first principles, many of the morals on those old lists. Some of the morals correspond to one of the basic types of cooperation (as in the case of courage), while others correspond to component parts of a basic type (as in the case of gratitude, which is a component of reciprocity).

But what about the other moral values mentioned, such as filial piety and honour? Do we have to keep adding new types of cooperation to the list in order to account for them? Not necessarily. In a recent paper, my colleagues and I show how morality is a combinatorial system in which the seven basic moral ‘elements’ combine to form a much larger number of more complex moral ‘molecules’.

A combinatorial system is one in which a relatively small number of simple things are combined to form a relatively large number of more complex things. Combinatorial systems are ubiquitous. Matter is a combinatorial system. DNA is a combinatorial system. Language, cuisine and music are all combinatorial systems. And these systems are incredibly powerful. For example, the 118 elements of the periodic table can combine to form millions of chemical compounds. And a mere six musical notes can be combined to generate trillions of tunes.

Could morality be such a system? As an initial test of the idea, we hypothesised possible moral molecules that combined each pair of moral elements, and then tried to find examples of them in the popular and professional literature. In each case, we succeeded.

For example, the idea that ‘you ought to defer to your superiors’ and the idea that ‘you ought to love your family’ combine to form the idea that ‘you ought to be especially deferential to senior members of your family’. This is the basis of filial piety. The idea that ‘you ought to be heroic’ and the idea that ‘you ought to avenge injuries’ combine to form the idea that ‘you ought to display your heroism by avenging injuries’. This is the basis of honour. And so on. Other molecules include: blood revenge (love of family + reciprocity); friendship (loyalty + reciprocity); patriotism (loyalty + heroism); confession (reciprocity + deference); turn-taking (reciprocity + fairness); restitution (reciprocity + property rights); mercy (heroism + fairness), and more.

So, morality does indeed seem to be a combinatorial system. And as such, the theory of ‘morality as cooperation’ can explain not only seven basic types of morality (elements), but also their 21 pairwise combinations (molecules). This combinatorial system incorporates even more of the morals on those old lists, and extends beyond them, to capture ever more obscure and recondite moral concepts (such as gavelkind, the equal division of an estate among heirs).

The theory provides a new way of explaining how and why morals vary across cultures

Of course, the number of possible molecules doesn’t end there. There could also be combinations of three, four, five, six or seven elements – as many as 127 in total. For example, if we combine the idea of love of family with the idea of honour (reciprocity + heroism) then we get the concept of family honour – or izzat, as they call it in parts of South Asia.

There can also be combinations of the uncooperative immoral elements – neglect, betrayal, cheating, cowardice, disrespect, unfairness and theft. For example, disrespecting a superior by stealing from them is poaching. Again, there are 127 possible combinations of these seven immoral elements. And, there can be combinations of moral and immoral elements – for example, solidarity (among a group of subordinates) to defy superiors equals mutiny – making a total of 2,186 possible mixed-valence molecules.

Moral elements might combine in different proportions, too. For example, if friendship is equal parts mutualism and reciprocity, then perhaps comradeship is two parts mutualism and one part reciprocity. The mathematics of permutations tells us that there might be millions of such molecules.

Now, it is not a prediction of the theory that all of these moral possibilities will be actualities. It is an empirical endeavour to find out which of these many possible morals actually exist and to explain why. To track those efforts, we have created a document called ‘The Periodic Table of Ethics’ that covers the 127 positive molecules. (See, for example, new entries on manrent, storge, irredentism and escheat.) Readers are welcome to try to add to or improve upon our suggested molecules, fill in the remaining gaps, and come up with counterexamples that challenge the theory.

This molecular approach to morality might also help to discover solutions to some long-standing philosophical puzzles. For example, is morality innate or acquired? Is it universal or culturally particular? Well, why not both? Perhaps elements are innate and universal, whereas molecules are acquired and particular. Perhaps different peoples in different times and places have forged new combinations of moral concepts, to create new and unique moral systems. Thus, the theory provides a new way of explaining how and why morals vary across cultures.

The theory of ‘morality as cooperation’ provides a systematic taxonomy of morals and suggests that there will be as many types of morality as there are types of cooperation (and their combinations). It organises familiar moral concepts and alerts us to unfamiliar ones. It could perhaps even spur the creation of new moral concepts – novel combinations that we’ve yet to invent or imagine. In this way, the theory provides us at last with a scientific guide for how to be good.

Save

Share

Post

Email

1 November 2021