The pupil of the eye opens a remarkable window into the mind

by Christoph Strauch, experimental psychologist

From mental effort to the content of a person’s imagination, these tiny apertures reveal far more than you might realise

To read someone’s intentions – or to know what they are feeling – we look instinctively into their eyes. In moments of negotiation or intimacy, we have an intuitive sense that the eyes convey more than their outward beauty. They offer something deeper, something unspoken yet powerful.

I am a psychologist and pupillometrist. I study the pupils – those small, seemingly black openings in the eye that expand and contract continuously. Research in my field shows the eyes really are a window into the psyche – in fact, they might be even more revealing than you realise.

You might have been taught that pupil-size changes are nothing more than a reflex response to changes in brightness. While it’s true that the pupils expand as it gets darker and constrict as brightness increases, they can give away much more information than that, subtly reflecting a person’s mental effort, their feelings – even the content of their imagination.

At a physiological level, the size of the pupil is determined by the two interacting muscles of the iris (the coloured part of the eye). These muscles are controlled by the parasympathetic and sympathetic nervous systems – these are the two opposing branches of the body’s automatic control centre that manage relaxation and alertness, respectively.

Given this basic anatomy, perhaps the least surprising information revealed by the pupils is how awake someone is – their pupils will be smaller when they’re tired and larger when they’re wide awake. If you’ve ever looked into the bathroom mirror after a long tiring day, perhaps you have noticed how small your own pupils can be.

Any increase in mental effort, no matter how small, is accompanied by a measurable increase in pupil size

As well as revealing how awake someone is, their pupils can also tell you how intensely they are exerting mental effort. The Nobel laureate Daniel Kahneman (re)discovered in the 1960s that the more digits a participant memorised, the larger their pupils became (I say rediscovered because a largely forgotten early literature anticipated many modern findings in this field). This effect isn’t bound to digits – any greater demand on memory will make the pupils dilate. The phenomenon is so reliable that it can be used to distinguish people based on their intelligence. For instance, if you give people arithmetic tasks of equal difficulty, those showing less pupil dilation (a sign of less mental effort) will be the ones who also tend to score higher on academic tests.

This connection between pupil size and mental effort is both sensitive and broad. Any increase in mental effort, no matter how small, is accompanied by a measurable increase in pupil size. Moreover, any kind of increased effort can cause the change – be that sexual interest, controlling body movements (or even just imagining making movements), being touched by others or experiencing intense emotions, such as surprise.

Perhaps even more remarkable is that changes in pupil size can reveal what a person is attending to mentally. This is because the pupil’s reflexive response to light can be triggered not only by what a person looks at directly, but also by what they are choosing to attend to with their mental spotlight (even if their gaze is directed elsewhere).

For instance, imagine staring straight ahead, but having a bright object such as a candle on one side of you, and a dark object such as a cup on the other. While keeping your eyes still, you can choose to attend covertly either to the candle or the cup. Your pupils will then respond to the attended brightness, without your eyes moving even a tiny bit – thus giving away where you are choosing to focus your mental spotlight.

But perhaps the most astonishing fact about pupil size is that it can reveal the content of a person’s imagination. In a 2013 study from the University of Oslo, participants imagined familiar scenarios, such as a bright sky, the sun, or a darkened room, all the while looking at the same grey background. Their pupils were smaller when imagining the bright scenes, compared with the dark scenes. This finding mimics a case report from the 1920s of a participant who lost his eyesight as a child, but who many years later was still able to recall the image of a glaring sun and, when he did so, his pupils became noticeably smaller.

When participants with aphantasia recalled the bright and dark shapes, their pupil sizes stayed unaltered

Because the pupils can give away what we’re imagining, this means they can also be used to study the vividness of a person’s mental imagery. A 2022 study focused on aphantasia, the self-reported inability to form visual imagery. Aphantasia is challenging to study for psychologists because it’s difficult to objectively verify a person’s subjective experiences. Are they truly unable to form imagery? Or do they simply label their imagery experiences differently than most other people?

Studying pupil size offers a brilliant way to explore these questions. In one task of the 2022 study that used a similar logic as the earlier research on the link between visual imagery and pupil size, participants with and without self-reported aphantasia first saw shapes of different brightnesses and later recalled these shapes while looking at an empty computer screen.

When the control participants recalled the shapes, the researchers saw they had a smaller pupil size when recalling the bright shapes than for dark shapes, suggesting that they were seeing the bright shapes in their mind’s eye. By contrast, when the participants with self-reported aphantasia recalled the bright and dark shapes, their pupil sizes stayed unaltered. This implies that they couldn’t visually reproduce the shapes and so there was no effect on their pupils. It’s as if their pupils were giving the researchers a window into the private world of their minds.

I hope I have convinced you that pupil-size changes are a fascinating marker of many aspects of the psyche. My academic peers and I study the finest pupil-size changes with expensive and specialised equipment. But if you’re intrigued to find out more, you can get started doing pupillometry with a very simple item of equipment called a Haab pupillometer.

With this and other simple tools, the Swiss ophthalmologist Otto Haab managed to measure pupil-size changes so well that he discovered the way that covert shifts in attention can affect the pupil’s light response – more than 100 years before its description by contemporary researchers. If you want to give it a try, the principle is easy:

  1. Print out the image below on A4 paper, then cut out the pupillometer as a rectangle (so make sure to leave the semi-circles in).
  2. Next, hold the basic pupillometer next to your participant’s eye (or your own eye if you are using a mirror). Select the half-circle that most closely matches your pupil size.
  3. Focus on a single spot with constant brightness and avoid blinking. Do this 10 times and measure your pupil size in each case. Take the average. This is your baseline measure.
  4. Now repeat: do the same another 10 times or so, but additionally perform some tricky mental calculations while you go along, such as multiplying the numbers 13 and 14, or 8 and 13. Write down all measurements (or ask a helper to write them down) and, again, take the average.
  5. Did you observe larger average pupil sizes for the calculation task than the baseline? If so, you just demonstrated the effect of mental effort on pupil size, replicating a 1964 paper in Science, using your own pupillometer.

Remember, changes might be subtle, but even minor pupil expansion reflects heightened mental effort. Congratulations, you’ve just joined the ranks of pupillometrists!

A black gauge with white semicircular cutouts labelled 2.0 to 9.5 mm; measures in millimetres when printed on A4.

A Haab pupillometer; cut it out in a rectangle, then you can use the half circles as a way to measure pupil size

Even in the age of brain scans, pupillometry allows fascinating and even unique insights into our psyches. That’s why pupillometry is arguably more popular today than ever. Doing pupillometry is easy compared with brainscans. A brainscan is very expensive, participants can’t move in the scanner because it ruins the data, and data analysis is error-prone because of its high complexity. If the additional details a brainscan provides aren’t needed to answer a research question, an easier method such as pupillometry is therefore preferable.

Furthermore, pupil responses are quite reliable, meaning that one does not need to measure as long and you can still get good results. In fact, even some brainscan studies additionally measure the pupils because of this. The possibility to study what someone is imagining, and how vividly, is why I first fell in love with this field – that little dark opening to the eye really is a very special window.

Syndicate this idea

Explore more

A couple dancing on stage, the photo focusing on their feet and shadows, with dramatic lighting and a blue spotlight.
DANCE

Dance showed me the untapped power of our attention muscle

Through tango, I sharpened attentional skills that make any moment richer. But these can be honed on or off the dancefloor

by Sara Melzer

Sketchbook pages with colourful abstract faces, featuring torn paper flaps over parts of the drawings, mixed media artwork.

Steven has a rare insight into faces – because he cannot recognise them

Directed by Steven Fraser

Page from a book with printed text about society and handwritten notes in the margin, including ’True but only in part‘.

The value of scribbling in the margins

Marginalia is far from inessential. It would be a shame if it died off in the digital age

by Richard Fisher

Black and white photo of a group of people dancing outdoors, with a man in a striped shirt in the foreground.

How to thrive as a highly sensitive person

In this noisy world, being highly sensitive is a challenge. But learn to manage overwhelm and you can reap the upsides too

by Jadzia Jagiellowicz

A young boy smiling, resting his chin on his hands, looking at a marshmallow on a table.

What the marshmallow test got wrong about child psychology

Self-control, grit, growth mindset – trendy skills won’t transform children’s lives, but more meaningful interventions can

by Tyler W Watts

Photo of uniformed men standing outside portable toilets, one gestures invitingly.

Does progress seem slower when you constantly check on it?

Research on how we perceive the rate of change shows how you can be strategic about goal tracking and boost your motivation

by André Vaz

Close-up photo of a hand carving a pencil with a small knife, focusing on the pencil’s tip and wood shavings.

There’s joy in doing a job right. Just ask this artisanal pencil-sharpener

Directed by Kenneth Price

Photo of a man in profile with glasses, eyes closed, head tilted back under a structure with a grid-like roof.

How slow breathing calms down your brain

Researchers studied the effect of slow breathing on people’s brain activity while they experienced anticipatory anxiety

by Christian Jarrett